View full metadata

DOI data

10.3280/RIP2016-002001

Serial Article

Cite as

Journal Data
Journal Issue Data
Serial Article Data
Reference list of 10.3280/RIP2016-002001

Unstructured Citation

Antonietti, A., Confalonieri, E., & Marchetti, A. (Eds.) (2014). Reflective thinking in educational settings: A cultural framework. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139198745


Unstructured Citation

Antonietti, A., Liverta-Sempio, O., & Marchetti, A. (Eds.).(2006). Theory of mind and language in developmental contexts. New York, NY: Springer.

https://doi.org/10.1007/b106493


Unstructured Citation

Astington, J.W. (2003). Sometimes necessary, never sufficient: False-belief understanding and social competence. In B. Repacholi & V. Slaughter (Eds.), Individual differences in theory of mind: Implications for typical and atypical development. New York, NY: Psychology Press.


Unstructured Citation

Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). Precursors to a theory of mind: Understanding attention in others. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.


Unstructured Citation

Bernstein, D.M., Thornton, W.L., & Sommerville, J.A. (2011). Theory of mind through the ages: older and middle-aged adults exhibit more errors than do younger adults on a continuous false belief task. Experimental Aging Research, 37(5), 481-502.

https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2011.619466


Unstructured Citation

Bretherton, I., McNew, S., & Beeghly-Smith, M. (1981). Early person knowledge as expressed in gestural and verbal communication: When do infants acquire a ‘theory of mind’? In M. E. Lamb & L. R. Sherrod (Eds.), Infant social cognition (pp. 33-373). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.


Unstructured Citation

Bruner, J. (1996). The culture of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.


Unstructured Citation

Bruner, J. (2002). Making stories. Law, literature, life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.


Unstructured Citation

Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.


Unstructured Citation

Fabio, R.A., Antonietti, A., & Pravettoni, G. (2008). Benefici e costi dei processi di automatizzazione dell’attenzione visiva. Ricerche di Psicologia, 30(3), 109-120.


Unstructured Citation

Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (2001). Attaccamento e funzione riflessiva. Milano: Raffaello Cortina Editore.


Unstructured Citation

Kuhn, D. (2000). Theory of mind, metacognition, and reasoning: A life-span perspective. In P. R. Mitchell (Ed.), Children’s reasoning and the mind. Hove, UK: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis.


Unstructured Citation

Lohmann, H., Tomasello, M., & Meyer, S. (2005). Linguistic Communication and Social Understanding. In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 245-265). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0012


Unstructured Citation

Maugham, W. S. (1933). Ah king: Six stories. London: William Heinemann. Moron, M. (1997). Unpublished MA Thesis.


Unstructured Citation

Siegal, M. (1997). Knowing children: Experiments in conversation and cognition. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.


Unstructured Citation

de Villiers, J. G. (2005). Can Language Acquisition Give Children a Point of View? In J. W. Astington & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for the ory of mind (pp.186-219). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press

https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195159912.003.0010